Cognitive Neuroscience & Brain Mapping
Thursday, May 2, 2013
Summary and Blog Stats
Our service learning grid project focused on contributing to Cognitive Neuroscience research by utilizing the computational power of multiple computers to increase processing efficiency and data collection. We connected to the Mindmodeling@Home grid project. Through our interview with Dr. Sturgill and evaluation of our research paper, we were able to gain a better understanding of the evolution of the human brain and its cognitive capabilities through morphological changes. As a result, we were able to gain a better appreciation for our cognitive abilities and how the modern human skull and brain has evolved from that of our recent ancestors and relatives such as Neanderthal.
Units contributed to the grid: 18, 528.10 as of March 2, 2013
Currently ranked as number 621 (based on average use) out of 2000+ contributors.
Our grid completed a record-breaking 52 jobs, roughly 13,000 days worth of computation, in less than one month.
Friday, April 19, 2013
Heritability, Vg, Ve, and Cranial Morphology in Humans and Neandertals.
1. Both your
interview and your article mentioned the complex factors involved in Neandertal
brain/cranial morphology. What is
the h2 of facial form (or facial size) thought to be? What does this tell you about the VE
and VG of cranial morphology?
Both your interview and your article mentioned the complex factors involved in Neandertal brain/cranial morphology. What is the h2 of facial form (or facial size) thought to be? What does this tell you about the VG and VE of cranial morphology?
Both your interview and your article mentioned the complex factors involved in Neandertal brain/cranial morphology. What is the h2 of facial form (or facial size) thought to be? What does this tell you about the VG and VE of cranial morphology?
When considering any trait in an organism, evolutionary
biologist must try and determine how much of a trait is dependant on
heritability (h2). This narrow sense ability determines a ratio or
percentage that can be attributed to genetic factors (VG) by diving
the genetic variation by the total phenotypic variation.
For cranial size, I found three different numbers for heritability in humans, and they are as follows (taken from Martinez-Abadias’s “Heritability of human cranial dimensions: comparing the evolvability of different cranial regions.”):
For cranial size, I found three different numbers for heritability in humans, and they are as follows (taken from Martinez-Abadias’s “Heritability of human cranial dimensions: comparing the evolvability of different cranial regions.”):
Height: h2 = 0.34
Length: h2 = 0.32
Breadth: h2 = 0.28
When these numbers are analyzed as percentages, it is easy to see that cranial size in humans has low heritability. In other words, about two-thirds of variability in human skull size is due to environmental factors (VE); genetic makeup only accounts for about one-third of variability in human skull size.
Our paper discusses a couple different environmental factors that may contribute to the variance in skull morphology, and they are climate, locomotor behaviors of the species, and genetic drift.
Neandertals were thought to live in northern Europe, an area
of mostly cold climate. However,
when comparing morphology of Neandertals to humans today, they were most
similar to those skulls of sub-Saharan Africa and so this theory was not
exactly supported as the main environmental factor. They do make mention of
nasal features corresponding to morphologies found in high-altitude organisms,
which could give some (if only little) credibility to that theory.
Dental loading is also another theory considered to explain
skull morphology, because the anterior teeth have more wear than posterior
teeth. This dental loading would have to be considered an adaptive trait on
which natural selection would have acted for many generations. On investigation
of bite forces though, modeling shows that Neandertal bites were not
particularly high and therefore provides a problem for supporting this theory
of adaptive dental loading.
Genetic drift seems to be the most supported hypothesis in
his paper, as the author ran a simulation using quantitative genetics and was
unable to reject any statistical results. Further, he argues that fossil
records support the genetic drift theory even more by stating that skull
features do not appear all at once but rather gradually accumulate over the
years.
Tuesday, April 16, 2013
2. Two of
the three hypotheses proposed to explain the evolution of Neandertal
cranial features are adaptive (natural selection), and one is “neutral”
(genetic drift). Explain these
competing hypotheses and why the authors support the latter,
non-ultra-Darwinian view.
This article presents three hypotheses for the evolutionary explanation for Neandertal cranial features: adaptation to cold climates, adaptation to anterior dental loading, and genetic drift. The first hypothesis, claiming adaptation to cold climates, is based on the observation that the geographic range of the Neandertals was centered in northern Europe, which was considerably cooler at the time of the Neandertals. However, just because they experienced colder temperatures does not require that their cranial features are adaptations to these colder conditions. Since most of these type of hypotheses focused on the nasal region. Studies have been done on the internal nasal dimensions. The narrow superior internal nasal dimensions, tall nasal apertures, and progecting nasal bridges are typically found in high latitude recent humans and could be adaptations to cold climates. However, the Neandertal nasal region does not appear to be an adaptation to cold climates and if they were adapting to cold similarly to presen-day humans, then climatic adaptation is not a likely explanation for their cranial form.
This article presents three hypotheses for the evolutionary explanation for Neandertal cranial features: adaptation to cold climates, adaptation to anterior dental loading, and genetic drift. The first hypothesis, claiming adaptation to cold climates, is based on the observation that the geographic range of the Neandertals was centered in northern Europe, which was considerably cooler at the time of the Neandertals. However, just because they experienced colder temperatures does not require that their cranial features are adaptations to these colder conditions. Since most of these type of hypotheses focused on the nasal region. Studies have been done on the internal nasal dimensions. The narrow superior internal nasal dimensions, tall nasal apertures, and progecting nasal bridges are typically found in high latitude recent humans and could be adaptations to cold climates. However, the Neandertal nasal region does not appear to be an adaptation to cold climates and if they were adapting to cold similarly to presen-day humans, then climatic adaptation is not a likely explanation for their cranial form.
Another characteristic of
Neandertals is that they tend to have more worn anterior teeth than posterior
ones. In addition to that, these anterior
teeth showed a high incidence of enamel chipping, microfractures, and microstriations
on the labial surfaces which suggest that Neandertals used their mouths like a
vise. The second hypothesis, the
anterior dental loading hypothesis, states that the facial form of the
Neandertal are adaptations to dissipate the high mechanical loads produced by
such behavior. However, the article
states that since the facial features of Neandertals appear early in
development they cannot be direct mechanical responses to anterior dental
loading. The article goes on to say that
instead, these would have to adaptations produced by natural selection after
the species performed the behavior for several generations. Another issue the author has with this
hypothesis is that biomechanical modeling suggests that Neandertals were not
able to produce high bite forces, at least not high enough to promote any
resistance, and thus their cranial form cannot be adapted to resisting high
bite forces if they were incapable of producing them.
The author favors the genetic
drift hypothesis for many reasons. First
is that Neandertals and modern human populations became isolated from one
another around 350,00 years ago. This
would have caused the two to diverge from each other, even in the absence of
natural selection through events such as changing allele frequencies. They tested this hypothesis and estimated
that both groups diverged 311,000 to 435,00 years ago, which closely matches
dates derived from ancient Neandertal and extant human DNA sequences, which
they say is expected if genetic drift is responsible for the cranial
divergence. Another strong piece of
supportive information the author brings up comes from fossil records. He says that Neandertal features, like modern
human features, did not appear all at once, rather, they gradually accumulated
over hundreds of thousands of years. This
is the expected pattern if genetic drift were responsible.
Monday, April 15, 2013
3. In an evolutionary sense, why is it informative to study
dental and cranial morphology in hyraxes or non-human primates?
Studying
dental and cranial morphology in non-human primates can be informative because
it allows us to learn about and describe general aspects of the body mass, diet,
and behavior of a fossil primate. Paleoanthropologists use the comparative
method to understand morphological adaptations in fossil primates; meaning they
base their inferences on the fossil primates by comparing them to studies of
form and function of extant primates. For example, if the fossil primate and
extant primate seem to have similar dental characteristics then you can infer
that they may have eaten the same types of foods. Dental characteristics are
related to more than just diet though, they also help determine the body mass
of primates. Large-bodied primates tend to have relatively large teeth and
paleoanthropologists hypothesize that correlates to body mass in extant
primates also hold for extinct primate taxa. So, teeth can tell us about the
size and weight of fossil primates and extant primates.
As
mentioned before, dental correlates to diet in fossil (and extant) primates,
specifically enamel thickness and dental morphology. Enamel thickness reflects
differences in the physical properties of foods eaten by primates. For example,
species with a diet composed of hard, gritty food such as seeds tend to have
thick enamel. Species such as folivores eat leafy plant foods and tend to have
thin enamel. The anterior dentition of many primates serves in the initial
preparation of food for chewing. The functional signal for incisor and canines
is seen in primates that use their teeth on hard substances such as wood, bark,
and fruit. The projections of the incisors can also tell you how the primate
eats its food. On the other hand, a primate that eats leafy materials passes
its food directly to its premolars and molars (back teeth). It’s easier to chew
up leaves with your posterior dentition than with your front canines and
incisors.
Pic on Left: Chimps are omnivorous (so they
have characteristics of both plant eating molars and sharp canines and
incisors) and their diet mostly consists of fruits and plants, insects, and sometimes
small monkeys (baby bushbucks, young baboons) and baby mammals (meat is only 5%
of diet, males hunt for meat more than females). ( http://www.buzzle.com/articles/chimpanzee-diet.html)
Australopithecus
afarensis are one of the early human forms from 3.85 and 2.95 million years
ago in Eastern Africa. They had apelike face proportions (flat nose, strongly
projecting lower jaw) and braincase (small brain, 1/3 of modern human brain),
long arms with curved fingers for climbing trees, small canine teeth, and lived
both in trees and on ground which helped them survive for almost 1 million
years as the climate and environments changed. They mainly ate a plant-based
diet; the remains of their teeth indicate they ate soft, sugar rich fruits but
their tooth size and shape suggest they could have eaten hard, brittle foods
too (fall back foods for when seasons didn’t have fruit possibly?). (http://humanorigins.si.edu/evidence/human-fossils/species/australopithecus-afarensis)
There
is some experimental evidence that lifetime behavior differences, specifically
which foods are eaten, can influence cranial form. Experiments on rock hyraxes
investigated the effects of food processing on cranial growth and form.
Lieberman and colleagues found that the maxillary molars of rock hyraxes are
positioned directly behind the orbits as in humans, which make them more
appropriate models for human mechanical loading patterns than some other prognathic
primates. They found that diet and perhaps certain behaviors can influence
facial size.
Picture on the left shows lateral views of human (top),
rock hyrax (middle), and baboon (bottom) adult skulls scaled to same length.
The entire molar row lies beneath or posterior to the plane of the
orbits (dashed line) in humans and hyraxes, but not in baboons (which are a
particularly prognathic primate). (http://ars.els-cdn.com/content/image/1-s2.0-S004724840400051X-gr1.jpg)
Cranial
morphology provides insight on both diet and locomotion in fossil primates.
Paleoanthropologists can gain information on the relative bite force involved
in mastication, or chewing. Primates that feed on hard materials (i.e. seeds,
fibrous plant parts) tend to have larger muscles and muscle attachments on
their skull and mandible.
Information
on activity patterns and locomotion can be determined by looking at the
relative size of the eye orbits compared to the overall size of the skull.
Nocturnal primates tend to have relatively larger eye orbits compared to their
skulls and diurnal primates have relatively small orbits compared to the skull.
The location of the foramen magnum approximates body posture and locomotion. In
tetrapods, the foramen magnum tends to be located more posterior on the head
but in upright primates, it is located directly under the skull.
Studying
dental and cranial information in non-human primates and hyraxes is beneficial
and informative to understanding the behaviors and environments of early human
forms and how they evolved into the modern human form that we know today. By
studying changes in dental morphology throughout non-human primates, we can
infer their diet and methods of eating and compare their ways to humans. We can
study when these changes occurred and in what species and possibly link the
changes to when there was a significant climate or environmental change that
caused species to evolve and new species to form. The same goes for studying
cranial morphology. By comparing skulls between non-primates and humans, we can
see the differences and similarities and then try to figure out why the
differences that are there exist and possible explanations for the differences.
In our
paper, Weaver explains that there are three main evolutionary explanations for
Neanderthal cranial morphology including, adaptation to cold climates (most
have been found in northern Europe), adaptation to anterior dental loading, or
genetic drift. Neanderthal features didn’t all appear at once; they gradually
accumulated over a period of 300,000 years. A similar pattern may explain the
appearance of modern human features. Neanderthals could have been isolated by
genetic drift and this could have led to our modern human form. Studying dental
and cranial morphology allows us to figure out a time frame of when these
changes could have occurred, why, and explain why we look and behave the way we
do today.
Reference: "Primate Origin", http://www.pearsoned.ca/highered/showcase/lehman/media/lehm_ch05.pdf
4. Define autapomorphy and
compare and contrast it with symplesiomorphy and synapomorphy, citing a
Neandertal skeletal example of each.
Autapomorphy is one type of an uninformative character
as a derived character state that is restricted to a single terminal taxon in a
data set. While synapomorphic traits are
also derived character states, synapomorphies are shared traits and are used to
define monophyletic groups. In other
words, synapomorphic traits are seen in decedents of a common ancestor, but not
in the ancestor itself. Autapomorphic traits
are used to distinguish organisms from others who are closely related, with a
recent common ancestor, while cladistics uses synapomorphies to help group
together taxa sharing a recent common ancestor as they tell us about
relationships within a group. Thus, autapomorphic and synapomorphic traits
are informative occurrences, while symplesiomorphic character states are
primitive features that are shared between a common ancestor and its descendants. Symplesiomorphic traits are therefore
phylogenetically uninformative and do not indicate details of relationships.
The article, The Meaning of Neandertal Skeletal
Morphology, summarized these trait classifications and their importance in
cladistics and evolutionary character state analysis, “the appearance of
derived features in the fossil record can pint to the action of directional
natural selection or genetic drift, and the retention of primitive features can
indicate stabilizing natural selection (Weaver and Klein).” A Neandertal
skeletal example of an autapomorphic trait is that Neandertals show a greater
convexity of the infraorbital plane, or in other words, the absence of
infraorbital concavity (Weaver and Klein) which is indicated by the a more
posterior placement of the inferior orbital margin. This convexity appears to be unique to the
Neandertal as Homo erectus shows a
flat infraorbital region and modern humans are defined by a concave condition
of the infraorbital plane (http://www.google.com/url?sa=t&rct=j&q=&esrc=s&source=web&cd=12&ved=0CDgQFjABOAo&url=http%3A%2F%2Fpages.nycep.org%2Fed%2Fdownload%2Fpdf%2FNMG%252024.pdf&ei=ySlxUcGCKoeMyAH_vYGACw&usg=AFQjCNEYQpwU_rJQpUtR44ZI-GCgMOzdtw&bvm=bv.45373924,d.aWc).
An example of a symplesiomorphic or shared ancestral
character state is the absence of a mental eminence, in other words the absence
of a chin (Weaver and Klein). The
absence of a mental eminence is a character state is a shared by the Neandertal
and its ancestors, unique to modern humans.
Upright walking is a synapomorphic character trait of Neandertal and its
close relatives with respect to Homo erectus
as the recent common ancestor.
5. What does Neandertal cranial morphology
have to do with cognitive neuroscience in modern humans?
As a relatively new scientific field, cognitive
neuroscience can gain a lot of understanding from the study of Neandertal cranial
morphology, gaining insight into the events that lead to our great specialization
and cognitive abilities. ‘Paleoneurology
is the only available tool to analyze and understand human brain evolution (www.emilianobruner.it/pdf/Paleoneuro03.pdf),
therefore understanding Neandertal
cranial morphology is the key to insight in the development of cognition and
resulting abilities.
By investigating the cranial morphology from
Neandertal to modern humans, there is the possibility of gaining insight into
how evolutionary modifications of anatomical traits altered the functional
abilities of the brain. Palaeoneurological
data of Neandertals might provide insight into the relationship between
internal brain organization and brain functions as the differences in the
structure of the cortex seen between that of modern humans and Neandertals
could indicate the reasons for our increased cognitive ability.
By studying the
differences in Neandertal and modern human brain anatomy, research may be able
to correlate an increasing number of relationships between anatomical regions
and function. Providing insight into function-location
relationships, can aid in the diagnosing and potential treatment of neurological
disorders and/or cognitive impairments including degenerative and functional diseases. For example, modern humans have increasingly
specialized language associated with the left hemisphere (http://psycnet.apa.org/?fa=main.doiLanding&doi=10.1037/0033-295X.96.3.492).
Morphological changes between Neandertal left hemisphere and the modern human
left hemisphere could provide insight into anatomical structures crucial to
language, and perhaps could provide information for the restoration of language
lost due to damage of the brain.
Saturday, March 23, 2013
Interesting Research Article!
Check out this paper, titled "The Meaning of Neanderthal Skeletal Morphology". The link is
http://www.jstor.org/stable/40485014 .
http://www.jstor.org/stable/40485014 .
Sunday, February 17, 2013
Our Interview with Dr. William Sturgill!
Dr. William Sturgill is a Professor of Psychology here at
Rockhurst and he teaches a multitude of classes, including courses like
Cognition, Cognitive Neuroscience, Psychology of Perception, Psychology of
Language, and more. His interests lie especially in cognitive neuroscience and
how the brain enables higher cognitive functions. Current research projects of
his include projects on the brain and humor, on the formation and maintenance
of an attentional set, and on visual perception of words. When we asked Dr.
Sturgill what made him decide to study cognitive neuroscience, his answer was
quite simple- it’s interesting! The brain is the most complex organ and an amazing
piece of evidence for evolution. There is so much that we have learned about
the brain, yet so many things are still unknown and may forever remain
mysteries.
To
prepare for our interview, the group prepared a handful of questions relating
to the evolution of the brain and comparing the human brain to the Neanderthal
and chimp brains, the evolution of thought, and artificial intelligence. We
planned on asking these questions one by one in typical interview fashion but
that’s not how it played out at all! Once we asked one question, the questions snowballed off of each other and we ending up covering a variety of topics! We started off by explaining the MindModeling
project to Dr. Sturgill and asked him if he thought we would ever really be
able to mimic human thought and other mental processes and be able to model the
mind for a computer program. He answered instantly with a big, fat NO! He said
that the brain is just way too complex for this kind of mind modeling program
to be feasible and there is still so much we don’t know about the brain, so how
we can model it if we don’t understand it completely yet ourselves!? Dr.
Sturgill started to talk about some of the connectionist models that people
have currently been working on since about the 1970s. Following is a recap of
some the things he talked about regarding cognition and connectionism.
Cognition
can be explained as the act of understanding and learning and involves mental
processes that take place in the brain. Connectionism explains cognition as the
process of multiple areas of the brain working together to process information
and think. Because each neuron in the brain has multiple dendrites, when an
action potential fires, synapses transfer across multiple areas of the brain. By
doing so, processing information can be learned through multiple perspectives.
For example, items could be processed by their color, texture, size, or other
features. The brain registers this information in the sensory part of the
brain. Following this process, your brain’s motor area may begin working for
you to act based on these features. For this all to occur, connectionists argue
that smaller units all work together in the brain to create action potential.
This process can be explained using complicated mathematical models found
online.
In
order for this to apply to artificial intelligence for the MindModeling project,
we have to consider the use of symbolism. Researchers over the last couple
decades have tried to mimic the human brain based on symbolism. With this method,
people type an input such as “brown” into the computer and connect it with
“dog.” Several features are added to the word dog so that if you ask the
computer a question like “what is brown and furry?” the computer will filter
out everything that isn’t brown and furry. In a sense, it is connecting the
word “dog” to “brown and furry.” The physical word “dog” symbolizes those two
characteristics. Critics do not believe this technique can ever be perfected to
mimic the human mind though because humans are so complex (http://www.cogsci.rpi.edu/~rsun/sun.encyc01.pdf).
The
Turing Test is used to measure whether a machine (such as a computer) can match
the human mind. With this test, a person sits there and types onto a keyboard a
conversation or topic starter. This input is then shared with a human being and
a machine made to mimic a human. The person then reads both responses (one from
a human and another as the machine) and tries to distinguish them. If the
person cannot tell the difference between the two, then the machine is said to
successfully have mimicked a human brain. Again, critics of this test do not
believe that this is technically mimicking the human brain because it is
simulated intelligence rather than real intelligence (http://www.turing.org.uk/publications/testbook.html).
As you
can see, there seems to be some people who think conditioning a computer with
input and output responses is similar to the way the brain works and therefore mimics the
brain, but this just isn’t the case. Computer programs, no matter how “smart”
they are, can’t make spontaneous decisions on their own like humans can and
therefore computers aren’t really mimicking the actions of the human brain. That’s
why it seems implausible to Dr. Sturgill that we would ever be able to create a
program of artificial intelligence that mimics the brain thought process and
other higher cognitive functions. After talking about connectionism and current
theories that claim to mimic the brain, the rest of our interview was spent
talking about the evolution of the brain. This topic in itself could be
discussed for days on end, not just the hour that we had but we got a
glimpse into the history of the brain. But before we talk about what Dr.
Sturgill had to say about this, we will briefly outline the structure of the
brain so you have a better idea of what we are talking about in regards to its
evolution.
In order for higher thinking and
creativity, there has to be connections and fluidity throughout the
cortex. The cerebrum, also known as the
cortex, is the part of the brain that is associated with tasks and higher brain
functioning such as thought and action.
The cerebrum is divided into four lobes: the frontal lobe (anterior part
of brain), parietal lobes (superior lateral sides of brain), occipital lobe
(posterior part of brain), and the temporal lobe (inferior lateral sides of
brain). Each lobe is given a specific
function to carry out within the process of thought and action. The frontal lobe is associated with
reasoning, planning, parts of speech, movement, emotions, and problem
solving. The parietal lobe is associated
with movement, orientation, recognition, and perception of stimuli. The occipital lobe is associated with visual
processing and the temporal lobe is associated with perception and recognition
of auditory stimuli, memory, and speech.
Within the central nervous
system (CNS, which includes the brain and spinal cord), nerves extend from your
brain to your face and from the spinal cord to the limbs and remaining parts of
your body. Sensory nerves gather
information from your environment (input) and send a signal to your brain to
perform the necessary action (output), which are carried out by motor nerves. The brain is essentially divided into a motor
section and sensory section and these two sections work together to allow us to
perform all of the actions we do on a daily basis. The brain is amazingly
interconnected this way. Each system
works separately but at the same time. Evolution has allowed them to work
together almost flawlessly. By that we mean interconnections are going on all the time
and we don’t even know it!
From
|
From http://www.sciguru.com/newsitem/11760/Compared-Neanderthals-modern-humans-have-better-sense-smell |
(http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/14972752).
Thus,
the frontal lobes contribute to our capability to be innovative as it requires
activation and communication between regions of the brain which usually do not possess
strong pathways. Thus it is possible as
a result of a more compact brain, whose versatility enables us to manipulate
the environment on a much larger scale than any other species. This is seen, according to Dr. Sturgill, in
the lifestyle of the Neanderthals, who lacked innovation which may have
resulted in their remaining in the freezing north with no other hunting methods
than the spear for thousands of years without looking for changes to improve
their quality of life.
Another
key to the evolution of the human brain with respect to innovation is the
importance of the evolution of thought.
Dr. Sturgill stated that speech is an adaptation of the vocalization of
thought, with communication as an exaptation as a result. Many believe that speech was adapted for communication
but Dr. Sturgill doesn’t think this is the case. Speech was adapted to express
all of the interconnections and thoughts and ideas going on in the brain. Without
the presence of frontal lobes, humans may not have established the increased
connections necessary for developing novel and alternative ideas, which through
the aid of communication, has allowed the evolutionary success of humans to
explore and expand our environment over the Neanderthals. If you want to take a look at the proposed evolutionary tree Homo sapiens brain, take a look below, it's pretty cool!
Overall, our interview with Dr. Sturgill went very well and was very informative! Like we mentioned before, there's so much history regarding the brain and it's evolution- that could be a class all on it's own probably. It was interesting to hear about the current models for the brain and what artificial intelligence currently exists. However, it seems unlikely that we will ever understand the brain enough to create an artificial brain that does the exact same things and thinks like us humans do. Decision making and higher cognitive functions are what make humans human so it makes sense that it is unlikely to make a model that exactly mimics us.
One of the most interesting parts of the interview to me was when we discussed the differences, or if there is a difference, between the 'mind' and physiological functioning of the brain. I felt this discussion showed the complexity and difficulty of studying cognitive neuroscience as there is very little we know about the brain and thought. In addition, the lack of concrete methods to study and/or understand cognition as well as varying interpretations about the definitions and interaction of the mind, soul, and neural firings. For example, we discussed the mind and neurological function as a 'chicken and egg' scenario, does the mind result from brain activity or does the mind influence our perception and cognition? Our interview was very interesting and showed us an introduction into the challenges and variety of areas to explore within cognitive neuroscience.
From http://neurophilosophy.wordpress.com/2006/08/07/499/ |
Overall, our interview with Dr. Sturgill went very well and was very informative! Like we mentioned before, there's so much history regarding the brain and it's evolution- that could be a class all on it's own probably. It was interesting to hear about the current models for the brain and what artificial intelligence currently exists. However, it seems unlikely that we will ever understand the brain enough to create an artificial brain that does the exact same things and thinks like us humans do. Decision making and higher cognitive functions are what make humans human so it makes sense that it is unlikely to make a model that exactly mimics us.
One of the most interesting parts of the interview to me was when we discussed the differences, or if there is a difference, between the 'mind' and physiological functioning of the brain. I felt this discussion showed the complexity and difficulty of studying cognitive neuroscience as there is very little we know about the brain and thought. In addition, the lack of concrete methods to study and/or understand cognition as well as varying interpretations about the definitions and interaction of the mind, soul, and neural firings. For example, we discussed the mind and neurological function as a 'chicken and egg' scenario, does the mind result from brain activity or does the mind influence our perception and cognition? Our interview was very interesting and showed us an introduction into the challenges and variety of areas to explore within cognitive neuroscience.
Subscribe to:
Posts (Atom)